search for


 

Impact of Childhood Maltreatment on Cognitive Function and Its Relationship With Emotion Regulation in Young Adults
J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2024; 35(3): 155-162
Published online July 1, 2024
© 2024 Korean Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

Min Seok Kim1*, Kyungmin Kim2,3*, Jihyun Nam4, Seung Jae Lee1,3, and Sang Won Lee2,3

1Department of Psychiatry, Kyungpook National University Hospital, Daegu, Korea
2Department of Psychiatry, Kyungpook National University Chilgok Hospital, Daegu, Korea
3Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Kyungpook National University, Daegu, Korea
4Bio-Medical Research Institute, Kyungpook National University Hospital, Daegu, Korea
Correspondence to: Sang Won Lee, Department of Psychiatry, Kyungpook National University Chilgok Hospital, 807 Hoguk-ro, Buk-gu, Daegu 41404, Korea
Tel: +82-53-200-3218, Fax: +82-53-200-3229, E-mail: leesangwon.psy@knu.ac.kr
*These authors contributed equally to this work.
Received January 8, 2024; Revised March 13, 2024; Accepted March 19, 2024.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0) which permits unrestricted non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Abstract
Objectives: Childhood maltreatment can negatively impact cognitive development, including executive function, working memory, and processing speed. This study investigated the impact of childhood maltreatment on cognitive function in young adults using various measurements, including computerized tests, and their relationship with emotional dysregulation.
Methods: We recruited 149 healthy individuals with and without maltreatment experiences and used the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV (WAIS-IV) and a computerized battery to analyze cognitive function.
Results: Both the WAIS-IV and computerized tests revealed that individuals with a history of childhood maltreatment had decreased cognitive function, especially in terms of working memory and processing speed. These individuals tended to employ maladaptive emotion regulation strategies. Among cognitive functions, working memory is negatively related to maladaptive emotion regulation strategies such as catastrophizing.
Conclusion: This study highlights the effects of childhood maltreatment on cognitive function in young adulthood. Moreover, the study suggests clinical implications of cognitive interventions for improving emotion regulation and cognitive function in individuals with a history of childhood maltreatment.
Keywords : Childhood maltreatment; Working memory; Processing speed; Rumination; Catastrophizing
INTRODUCTION

Childhood maltreatment includes physical, emotional, and sexual violence and neglect that might impair children’s health or impede their normal development. Additionally, childhood maltreatment is a remarkable global health concern [1]. Childhood maltreatment can have long-term consequences. Furthermore, childhood maltreatment is not only strongly correlated with the occurrence of mood disorders, including depressive disorders [2,3], but also with other mental health-related problems. Maltreatment experiences in early life alter various neurocognitive functions that can instill sensitivity to mental health problems [4]. Additionally, childhood maltreatment can have detrimental effects on the development of various brain regions involved in emotion or reward processing [5].

Maltreatment negatively affects cognitive development, including executive function, memory, working memory, and processing speed [6]. Structural changes in the hippocampus are typically associated with cognitive deterioration in maltreatment experiences [7,8]. Decreased hippocampal volume may be associated with susceptibility to psychological trauma [9] and is frequently observed in typical mental disorders such as major depression [10]. Therefore, maltreatment can significantly impair the development of various cognitive functions, potentially serving as a vulnerability factor for mental illnesses.

Various studies have examined the relationship between emotional processing impairment and maltreatment. Maltreatment experiences make recognizing emotional stimuli [11], maintaining appropriate emotions, and acquiring interpersonal skills difficult [12,13]. Emotional processing and cognitive function can have a mutual influence. Cognitive functions such as working memory and processing speed are reportedly related to the use of emotional control techniques such as reappraisal [14], and working memory training can increase emotional control [15]. Furthermore, emotions affect attention, learning, memory, and reasoning [16]. Difficulties in emotion regulation can be associated with a decrease in processing speed [17], which is a common cognitive impairment even in depression [18]. However, studies reporting the relationship of maltreatment experiences with emotion regulation difficulties, and cognitive impairment in young adults remain limited.

Hence, this study aimed to 1) demonstrate the effects of maltreatment on cognitive function in young adults using various cognitive measurements, including computerized tests, and 2) observe the relationship between maltreatment experiences and emotional dysregulation, as well as cognitive function.

METHODS

Participants

All participants were recruited through notices posted on the online bulletin boards of public or college Internet communities in Daegu, Korea. Online questionnaires were created using SurveyMonkey (https://www.surveymonkey.com). Healthy young adults with or without a history of maltreatment were recruited to participate in the study and facilitate an understanding of the research process. The exclusion criteria were acute or chronic psychiatric or medical diseases, history of head trauma, or history of drug or alcohol addiction. Of the 200 participants, 34 had a history of psychiatric diagnoses or treatment. Additionally, we excluded 15 participants who reported clinically significant depressive symptoms (score ≥25 on the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression [CES-D] scale) and two participants who were suspected of having major depressive disorder based on our screening tool (MINI Patient Health Survey) [19]. In total, 149 healthy participants were included in the analysis. This study was conducted in accordance with “The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki)” for experiments involving humans. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Kyungpook National University Chilgok Hospital (KNUCH2021-07-010-001). Informed consent was obtained from all participants upon enrollment in the study.

Measures

Cognitive assessments

This study utilized the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) [20], which is the most frequently used measurement tool to evaluate cognitive function in clinical practice. The WAIS-IV evaluates full-scale intelligence quotient (IQ) and employs four index scales: verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and processing speed.

We also used the Central Nervous System Vital Signs (CNSVS) computerized battery (https://www.cnsvs.com/) to assess cognitive function. The CNS-VS can detect cognitive abnormalities using a normative database [21]. The parameter consists of 10 tests, the results of which are combined to produce 15 separate standardized domain scores: composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, psychomotor speed, reaction time, cognitive flexibility, complex attention, processing speed, executive function, simple attention, motor speed, social acuity, reasoning, sustained attention, and working memory.

All cognitive assessment scores were standardized, with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15.

Psychological assessments

To evaluate childhood maltreatment experiences, we used the Early Trauma Inventory Self Report-Short Form (ETISRSF), a 27-item questionnaire evaluating four types of traumatic experiences: general trauma, physical, emotional, and sexual abuse [22]. The Korean version used in this study was standardized in 2012 [23]. We employed the ETISR-SF to categorize participants into maltreatment and non-maltreatment groups. Given that the standard cutoff score of the ETISR-SF has not been validated, we opted for a cutoff point of 8 for two reasons. First, the score of 8 exceeds 2 standard deviations from the mean scores of the nonclinical sample in a validation study conducted in Korea [23]. A previous study used this criterion to divide the participants into low and high trauma groups [24]. Second, a score of 8 was above the highest quartile in our samples. Although maltreatment prevalence differed according to sex, maltreatment type, and country of residence, the prevalence rate of self-reported maltreatment experiences was approximately 20%–30% [25].

To evaluate depressive symptoms, we used the Korean version of the CES-D, a 20-item questionnaire [26,27]. The level of difficulty in emotional control was assessed using the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) [28]. The DERS provides a comprehensive evaluation of the emotional difficulty in adults, with high total scores, indicating a greater degree of emotional difficulty. The Korean version of the scale was standardized in 2007 [29]. We also used the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ), a self-report questionnaire, to assess nine cognitive emotion regulation strategies (five adaptive and four less adaptive) [30]. The Korean version of the scale was validated in 2013 [31]. Additionally, we employed the Korean version of Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [32,33], and the scores of trait anxiety from the STAI were used to evaluate the participants’ level of anxiety.

Statistical analysis

An independent t-test was used to compare demographic, psychological, and emotion regulation differences as well as the outcomes of the cognitive assessments between the maltreatment and non-maltreatment groups. Additionally, an analysis of covariance was applied to control for the effects of depressive and anxiety symptoms on cognitive ability. Furthermore, the association between cognitive assessments and difficulties in emotional regulation was elucidated using Pearson’s and partial correlation coefficients, as well as multiple linear regression. Age, sex, education, and depressive symptoms measured by the CES-D were used as covariates in the partial correlation analysis. All analyses were conducted using Jeffrey’s Amazing Statistics Program 0.18.2 (https://jasp-stats.org).

RESULTS

Group differences in demographic and psychological characteristics

Age differed significantly between the two groups (t=2.43, p=0.016), whereas sex and education did not. The maltreatment group exhibited higher scores on the CES-D (t=3.19, p=0.002), DERS (t=2.66, p=0.009), and maladaptive strategies in the CERQ (t=2.55, p=0.012) than those displayed by the non-maltreatment group. Symptoms of anxiety and adaptive strategies for cognitive emotion regulation demonstrated no significant differences between the two groups. Table 1 presents detailed information on the participants’ demographic and psychological characteristics.

<caption>

Epidemiological and psychological characteristics in the maltreatment and non-maltreatment (control) groups

Characteristic Maltreatment (n=36) Non-maltreatment (n=113) t or χ2 p
Age (yr) 26.22±5.56 24.07±4.30 2.43 0.016
Sex 0.06 0.807
Male 19 57
Female 17 56
Education 1.63 0.202
High school graduate 20 76
College graduate 16 37
CES-D 15.69±4.61 13.11±4.12 3.19 0.002
STAI 46.61±8.22 43.42±9.49 1.81 0.073
DERS 92.94±21.37 83.96±16.34 2.66 0.009
CERQ (adaptive strategies) 65.58±14.27 64.36±13.55 0.46 0.643
CERQ (maladaptive strategies) 42.61±9.52 37.88±9.75 2.55 0.012
<fn id="t1fn1">

Data are presented as mean±standard deviation or numbers only. Adaptive strategies: The sum of the scores of acceptance, refocusing on planning, putting into perspective, positive refocusing, and positive reappraisal. Maladaptive strategies: The sum of the scores of self-blame, other-blame, rumination, and catastrophizing. CERQ, Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; CES-D, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression; DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory



Group differences in IQ measured by the WAIS-IV

The maltreatment group had lower full-scale IQ scores (F=16.43, p<0.001) on the WAIS-IV than those of the nonmaltreatment group. Among the WAIS-IV subscales, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and processing speed scores were lower after controlling for age, sex, education, and depressive symptoms in the maltreatment group than in the non-maltreatment group (all p-values <0.01). Verbal apprehension subscale scores were not significantly different between the two groups. Fig. 1 presents the detailed results.

Fig. 1. Group differences in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale IV scores after controlling the age, sex, education and Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scores. **p<0.01; ***p<0.001. IQ, intelligence quotient.

Group differences in cognitive function measured by computerized test

Among the cognitive function variables measured by the CNS-VS, the neurocognition index, psychomotor speed, reaction time, cognitive flexibility, processing speed, executive function, working memory, sustained attention, and motor speed were significantly lower in the maltreatment group than in the non-maltreatment group after controlling for age, sex, education, and depressive symptoms (all p-values <0.05). Table 2 lists the detailed statistical values.

<caption>

Group differences in cognitive function measured by computerized battery

Variables Maltreatment (n=36) Non-maltreatment (n=113) t value F value
Neurocognition index 98.61±14.20 105.80±10.10 -3.35** 9.22**
Composite memory 102.31±19.76 101.71±14.64 0.20 0.03
Verbal memory 104.78±16.26 102.52±16.50 0.72 0.40
Visual memory 99.89±18.84 100.77±14.59 -0.29 0.06
Psychomotor speed 103.14±12.48 115.02±14.88 -4.33*** 14.90***
Reaction time 90.83±15.79 99.81±16.36 -2.89** 7.40**
Complex attention 97.72±27.09 103.84±13.76 -1.79 2.15
Cognitive flexibility 99.19±19.02 108.48±16.19 -2.87** 7.34**
Processing speed 99.03±15.84 110.47±18.10 -3.40*** 9.62**
Executive function 99.94±17.55 108.81±15.95 -2.83** 7.45**
Social acuity 94.11±13.81 94.58±14.07 -0.17 0.27
Reasoning 107.89±12.20 110.88±11.41 -1.35 2.12
Working memory 99.06±10.07 105.85±10.50 -3.41*** 9.78**
Sustained attention 101.25±9.27 106.06±11.49 -2.29* 4.06*
Simple visual attention 91.81±54.53 103.31±13.49 -2.07* 2.73
Motor speed 104.06±11.29 111.60±12.22 -3.28** 8.28**
<fn id="t2fn1">

Data are presented as mean±standard deviation. *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; adjusted for age, sex, education, and CES-D scores, degree of freedom=143. CES-D, Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression



Relationship between cognitive function and emotion regulation patterns

A partial correlation analysis was performed after controlling for age, sex, education, and depressive symptoms. Significant correlations were identified between working memory and CERQ subscales including rumination (r'=-0.197, p=0.017) and catastrophizing (r'=-0.166, p=0.046) (Table 3).

<caption>

Pearson’s and partial correlation between cognitive functions and cognitive emotion regulations (n=149)

Verbal comprehension Perceptual reasoning Working memory Processing speed Full-scale IQ
Acceptance
r 0.103 -0.101 0.013 -0.121 -0.047
r' 0.111 -0.066 0.018 -0.146 -0.034
Refocusing on planning
r 0.136 -0.007 0.102 -0.003 0.074
r' 0.151 -0.001 0.082 0.025 0.087
Putting into perspective
r 0.063 -0.106 0.053 -0.024 -0.013
r' 0.061 -0.107 0.046 -0.009 -0.01
Positive refocusing
r <0.001 -0.087 0.083 0.035 0.005
r' 0.021 -0.118 0.069 0.033 -0.006
Positive reappraisal
r 0.080 -0.044 0.084 -0.127 -0.007
r' 0.086 -0.051 0.064 -0.099 -0.003
Self-blame
r 0.032 -0.067 -0.018 -0.053 -0.047
r' 0.022 -0.037 0.010 -0.067 -0.033
Other-blame
r 0.019 -0.03 -0.129 0.077 -0.021
r' 0.003 -0.036 -0.129 0.028 -0.046
Rumination
r -0.095 -0.164* -0.224** -0.087 -0.200*
r' -0.111 -0.116 -0.197* -0.104 -0.182*
Catastrophizing
r -0.123 -0.079 -0.198* -0.009 -0.139
r' -0.145 -0.037 -0.166* -0.023 -0.124
<fn id="t3fn1">

r, Pearson’s correlations. r', partial correlations. Conditioned on variables: age, sex, education, and CES-D scores. *p<0.05; **p<0.01. CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression; IQ, intelligence quotient



The multiple linear regression analysis demonstrated that among cognitive functions, working memory had a significant effect on catastrophizing (t=-2.055, p=0.042), a subscale of CERQ (Table 4).

<caption>

Multiple linear regression analysis (enter method) for explaining catastrophizing and rumination using cognitive measures (n=149)

Unstandardized coefficient 95% CI of B Standardized coefficient t p
B SE β
Catastrophizing
Verbal comprehension -0.010 0.036 -0.081 to 0.061 -0.027 -0.285 0.776
Perceptual reasoning 0.004 0.026 -0.048 to 0.056 0.014 0.138 0.891
Working memory -0.063 0.031 -0.124 to -0.002 -0.218 -2.055 0.042*
Processing speed 0.018 0.024 -0.029 to 0.065 0.069 0.740 0.461
Rumination
Verbal comprehension 0.010 0.036 -0.061 to 0.080 0.026 0.270 0.788
Perceptual reasoning -0.021 0.026 -0.072 to 0.031 -0.078 -0.793 0.429
Working memory -0.059 0.031 -0.120 to 0.001 -0.204 -1.931 0.056
Processing speed 0.005 0.024 -0.042 to 0.051 0.019 0.201 0.841
<fn id="t4fn1">

*p<0.05. CI, confidence interval; SE, standard error



Relationship between Korean version of WAIS-IV and maltreatment subtypes

In the partial correlation analysis, emotional abuse exhibited a negative relationship with working memory and processing speed, while physical abuse demonstrated a negative correlation with working memory function (all p-values <0.05). Supplementary Tables 1 and 2 display detailed results.

DISCUSSION

This study revealed that individuals who experienced childhood maltreatment exhibited decreased working memory and processing speed in both the WAIS-IV and computerized battery assessment compared to those without such experiences. The decline in working memory was associated with catastrophizing, a maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation mechanism, and was weakly correlated to rumination.

The level of maltreatment was generally higher in our study than that reported by a Korean non-clinical group in a previous study. The average score of the ETISR-SF was 4.42 in our study, whereas the validation study evaluated a non-clinical sample and reported a score of 2.33 [23]. Our participants were not previously diagnosed with mental disorders and were excluded from the screening tool if they were suspected of having major depressive disorder. However, considering that they demonstrated interest and volunteered for this study, the degree of maltreatment was presumed to be relatively higher than that in the healthy general population. Furthermore, the maltreatment group exhibited significantly more depressive symptoms than those demonstrated by the non-maltreatment group, consistent with the results of previous studies that identified a strong association between depressive symptoms and early-life interpersonal violations in non-clinical populations [34]. Additionally, the maltreatment group reported difficulties in emotion regulation and a heightened reliance on maladaptive strategies for emotion regulation, consistent with prior research findings in the general population [35,36]. These results indirectly support the reliability of classifying participants into maltreatment and nonmaltreatment groups.

Our study suggests that working memory and processing speed differed between the two groups. According to Cattell’s theory, general fluid intelligence is related to novel problem-solving skills [37]. Moreover, fluid intelligence is also significantly associated with working memory [38]. The increase in general fluid intelligence was mediated by changes in the processing speed, suggesting a connection between the two [39]. Chronic stress, such as childhood abuse, can damage hippocampal neurons, resulting in a volume reduction in the hippocampus [8]. These alterations in hippocampal structure and function contribute to impediments in learning and memory, which are linked to a decrease in general fluid intelligence, a crucial element of learning [40-42]. Therefore, our findings suggest that childhood maltreatment can cause abnormalities in brain structure or function through chronic stress, resulting in a decline in cognitive domains closely related to general fluid intelligence.

Among the four indices of the WAIS-IV, verbal comprehension displayed no difference between the maltreatment and non-maltreatment groups (Supplementary Tables 1-3). In the previous study, among the four indices, verbal comprehension has been suggested to be highly related to crystallized intelligence [43]. Crystallized intelligence refers to the cognitive domain associated with problem-solving skills that are already established within an existing culture and obtained through education. That encompasses knowledge previously acquired by individuals [44]. Therefore, in the case of verbal comprehension, it can be said that cognitive functions in different domains are evaluated compared to the other three indices. Unlike fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence encompasses the accumulation of knowledge and skills over a person’s lifetime [45]. Therefore, in this cognitive domain, functionality is expected to remain stable irrespective of individual stress events. The authors infer from this perspective that significant differences between maltreatment and non-maltreatment groups are unlikely, unlike in other cognitive domains.

Furthermore, according to the results of Pearson’s and partial correlation analyses of the cognitive functions and maltreatment subtypes, physical and emotional abuse was associated with a decrease in the cognitive domains related to general fluid intelligence (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). McLaughlin et al. [46] suggested that early psychosocial deprivation experiences, such as emotional neglect, can affect cognitive function due to a lack of various environmental and social interactions. Moreover, emotional and physical abuse were important traumatic events that could be associated with cognition in adults [47]. In another study conducted among adolescents, physical abuse negatively affected executive functioning and related cognitive domains [48]. Our results are consistent with these existing findings and are significant as they suggest that emotional and physical abuse in childhood can affect cognitive function in adulthood.

Our results indicated a significant relationship between working memory and maladaptive emotion regulation patterns, including rumination and catastrophizing. Working memory is the function of holding specific information at a moment and subsequently utilizing such information, serving as an internal resource for regulating emotions [49]. Various studies have demonstrated the beneficial effects of working memory in decreasing maladaptive emotion regulation patterns. In recent studies, emotion regulation ability improved after 20 days of working memory training, which might have enhanced the function of attention control measured by electroencephalography [15]. Additionally, 10 sessions of online cognitive control training can help reduce maladaptive emotion regulation patterns such as brooding [50,51] and rumination [52]. Furthermore, combining working memory training and mindfulness meditation can reduce excessive worry, possibly owing to improved attentional control [53]. The ability to update emotional information related to the working memory can modulate the emotion regulation efficacy [54]. Therefore, working memory related to cognitive control may play a pivotal role in emotion regulation compared to other cognitive abilities, as corroborated by our findings.

Our study has several limitations. First, the impact of childhood maltreatment may be related to other factors such as its duration and the age at which maltreatment began; however, these effects were not considered [55]. Second, factors such as parental intelligence, which can be directly related to cognitive function, were not evaluated. Third, the selection of our cutoff point may appear somewhat arbitrary; however, when we experimented with various cutoff points ranging from six to nine, we observed similar results (Supplementary Table 3). Finally, the participants were recruited from a community-based institution; thus, the results cannot be generalized to the entire population. In the future, additional reviews of multiple institutions in multiple regions may be required.

CONCLUSION

Despite the limitations mentioned above, our study suggests that childhood maltreatment experiences are associated with difficulties in emotion regulation in adulthood, and that maladaptive emotion regulation strategies could be easily applied by individuals with such experiences. In addition, childhood maltreatment experiences can negatively affect cognitive functions, such as processing speed and working memory, in adulthood. Additionally, decreased working memory function is linked to the use of maladaptive emotional control strategies. Appropriate cognitive training can help improve emotion regulation in adults who have experienced childhood maltreatment. However, further studies are required to confirm this hypothesis.

Acknowledgments

This manuscript is part of a Ph.D. thesis at Kyungpook National University (Min Seok Kim, the first author).

Supplementary Materials

The online-only Data Supplement is available with this article at https://doi.org/10.5765/jkacap.240001.

Availability of Data and Material

Data will be made available on an appropriate request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Sang Won Lee. Data curation: Min Seok Kim, Sang Won Lee, Jihyun Nam. Formal analysis: Sang Won Lee. Funding acquisition: Sang Won Lee. Investigation: Sang Won Lee, Kyungmin Kim. Methodology: Sang Won Lee. Resource: Sang Won Lee. Supervision: Sang Won Lee. Validation: Kyungmin Kim, Seung Jae Lee. Writing—original draft: Kyungmin Kim, Min Seok Kim, Sang Won Lee. Writing—review & editing: Kyungmin Kim, Min Seok Kim, Seung Jae Lee, Sang Won Lee.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea grants funded by the Korean government (2021R1F1A1049460 and RS-2023-00241248).

References
  1. Gilbert R, Widom CS, Browne K, Fergusson D, Webb E, Janson S. Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. Lancet 2009;373:68-81.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  2. Liu RT. Childhood adversities and depression in adulthood: current findings and future directions. Clin Psychol (New York) 2017;24:140-153.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  3. Lippard ETC, Nemeroff CB. The devastating clinical consequences of child abuse and neglect: increased disease vulnerability and poor treatment response in mood disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2020;177:20-36.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  4. McCrory EJ, Gerin MI, Viding E. Annual research review: childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative psychiatry - the contribution of functional brain imaging. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2017;58:338-357.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  5. Teicher MH, Samson JA. Annual research review: enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2016;57:241-266.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  6. Su Y, D'Arcy C, Yuan S, Meng X. How does childhood maltreatment influence ensuing cognitive functioning among people with the exposure of childhood maltreatment? A systematic review of prospective cohort studies. J Affect Disord 2019;252:278-293.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  7. Lee SW, Yoo JH, Kim KW, Kim D, Park H, Choi J, et al. Hippocampal subfields volume reduction in high schoolers with previous verbal abuse experiences. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci 2018;16:46-56.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  8. Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A. Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2012;109:E563-E572.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  9. Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci 2002;5:1242-1247.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  10. Opel N, Redlich R, Zwanzger P, Grotegerd D, Arolt V, Heindel W, et al. Hippocampal atrophy in major depression: a function of childhood maltreatment rather than diagnosis?. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014;39:2723-2731.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  11. Pollak SD, Messner M, Kistler DJ, Cohn JF. Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition. Cognition 2009;110:242-247.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  12. Shipman K, Edwards A, Brown A, Swisher L, Jennings E. Managing emotion in a maltreating context: a pilot study examining child neglect. Child Abuse Negl 2005;29:1015-1029.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  13. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Adaptive coping under conditions of extreme stress: multilevel influences on the determinants of resilience in maltreated children. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev 2009;2009:47-59.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  14. Opitz PC, Lee IA, Gross JJ, Urry HL. Fluid cognitive ability is a resource for successful emotion regulation in older and younger adults. Front Psychol 2014;5:609.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  15. Xiu L, Wu J, Chang L, Zhou R. Working memory training improves emotion regulation ability. Sci Rep 2018;8:15012.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  16. Tyng CM, Amin HU, Saad MNM, Malik AS. The influences of emotion on learning and memory. Front Psychol 2017;8:1454.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  17. Cristofanelli S, Baccini G, Centonze E, Colombesi A, Cariello M, Ferro L. Can dissociation mediate the relationship between emotional dysregulation and intelligence? An empirical study involving adolescents with and without complex trauma histories. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2023;20:1729.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  18. Nuño L, Gómez-Benito J, Carmona VR, Pino O. A systematic review of executive function and information processing speed in major depression disorder. Brain Sci 2021;11:147.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  19. Lim SW, Song HS, Oh YH, Shin HC, Cho KH, Chung SK, et al. A validation study of the abbreviated self-rated Korean version of MINI (MINI patient health survey). Anxiety Mood 2007;3:32-40.
  20. Weschler D. WAIS-IV administration and scoring manual. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation;2008.
    CrossRef
  21. Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS vital signs. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2006;21:623-643.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  22. Bremner JD, Bolus R, Mayer EA. Psychometric properties of the early trauma inventory-self report. J Nerv Ment Dis 2007;195:211-218.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  23. Jeon JR, Lee EH, Lee SW, Jeong EG, Kim JH, Lee D, et al. The early trauma inventory self report-short form: psychometric properties of the Korean version. Psychiatry Investig 2012;9:229-235.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  24. Shah AJ, Weeks V, Lampert R, Bremner JD, Kutner M, Raggi P, et al. Early life trauma is associated with increased microvolt T-wave alternans during mental stress challenge: a substudy of mental stress ischemia: prognosis and genetic influences. J Am Heart Assoc 2022;11:e021582.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  25. Moody G, Cannings-John R, Hood K, Kemp A, Robling M. Establishing the international prevalence of self-reported child maltreatment: a systematic review by maltreatment type and gender. BMC Public Health 2018;18:1164.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  26. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977;1:385-401.
    CrossRef
  27. Cho MJ, Kim KH. Diagnostic validity of the CES-D (Korean version) in the assessment of DSM-III-R major depression. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 1993;32:381-399.
  28. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2004;26:41-54.
    CrossRef
  29. Cho Y. Assessing emotion dysregulation: psychometric properties of the Korean version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Korean J Clin Psychol 2007;26:1015-1038.
    CrossRef
  30. Garnefski N, Kraaij V, Spinhoven P. Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Pers Individ Differ 2001;30:1311-1327.
    CrossRef
  31. Ahn HN, Lee NB, Joo HS. Validation of the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire in a Korean population. Korean J Couns 2013;14:1773-1794.
    CrossRef
  32. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the statetrait anxiety inventory (self-evaluation questionnaire). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press;1970.
  33. Hahn DW, Lee CH, Chon KK. Korean adaptation of Spielberger's STAI (K-STAI). Korean J Health Psychol 1996;1:1-14.
  34. Chu DA, Williams LM, Harris AW, Bryant RA, Gatt JM. Early life trauma predicts self-reported levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms in nonclinical community adults: relative contributions of early life stressor types and adult trauma exposure. J Psychiatr Res 2013;47:23-32.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  35. Burns EE, Jackson JL, Harding HG. Child maltreatment, emotion regulation, and posttraumatic stress: the impact of emotional abuse. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma 2010;19:801-819.
    CrossRef
  36. Garnefski N, van Rood Y, de Roos C, Kraaij V. Relationships between traumatic life events, cognitive emotion regulation strategies, and somatic complaints. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 2017;24:144-151.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  37. Cattell RB. Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin;1971.
    CrossRef
  38. van Aken L, Kessels RP, Wingbermühle E, van der Veld WM, Egger JI. Fluid intelligence and executive functioning more alike than different?. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2016;28:31-37.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  39. Fry AF, Hale S. Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychol Sci 1996;7:237-241.
    CrossRef
  40. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008;105:6829-6833.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  41. McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O'Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev 1995;102:419-457.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  42. Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 1992;99:195-231.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  43. Grégoire J. Measuring components of intelligence: mission impossible?. J Psychoeduc Assess 2013;31:138-147.
    CrossRef
  44. Baghaei P, Tabatabaee M. The C-Test: an integrative measure of crystallized intelligence. J Intell 2015;3:46-58.
    CrossRef
  45. Al-Bakri SAB, Salman AM. Fluid, crystallized intelligence and language proficiency: a correlational study. J Glob Sci Res 2020;9:834-844.
    CrossRef
  46. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Nelson CA. Neglect as a violation of species-expectant experience: neurodevelopmental consequences. Biol Psychiatry 2017;82:462-471.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  47. Wan Z, Rolls ET, Feng J, Cheng W. Brain functional connectivities that mediate the association between childhood traumatic events, and adult mental health and cognition. EBioMedicine 2022;79:104002.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  48. Spann MN, Mayes LC, Kalmar JH, Guiney J, Womer FY, Pittman B, et al. Childhood abuse and neglect and cognitive flexibility in adolescents. Child Neuropsychol 2012;18:182-189.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  49. Urry HL, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation in older age. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2010;19:352-357.
    CrossRef
  50. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EH, Vanderhasselt MA, Callewaert S, Demeyer I. The influence of cognitive control training on stress reactivity and rumination in response to a lab stressor and naturalistic stress. Behav Res Ther 2015;69:1-10.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  51. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EHW. Internet-delivered cognitive control training as a preventive intervention for remitted depressed patients: evidence from a double-blind randomized controlled trial study. J Consult Clin Psychol 2017;85:135-146.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  52. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EH, Demeyer I, Loeys T, Vanderhasselt MA. Effects of cognitive control training on the dynamics of (mal)adaptive emotion regulation in daily life. Emotion 2016;16:945-956.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  53. Course-Choi J, Saville H, Derakshan N. The effects of adaptive working memory training and mindfulness meditation training on processing efficiency and worry in high worriers. Behav Res Ther 2017;89:1-13.
    Pubmed CrossRef
  54. Pe ML, Raes F, Kuppens P. The cognitive building blocks of emotion regulation: ability to update working memory moderates the efficacy of rumination and reappraisal on emotion. PLoS One 2013;8:e69071.
    Pubmed KoreaMed CrossRef
  55. Kaplow JB, Widom CS. Age of onset of child maltreatment predicts long-term mental health outcomes. J Abnorm Psychol 2007;116:176-187.
    Pubmed CrossRef


October 2024, 35 (4)
Full Text(PDF) Free
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
Supplementary File
PubMed Central
Google Scholar Search

Social Network Service
Services
Close ✕


Stats or Metrics

Author ORCID Information

Funding Information