Savant syndrome refers to a condition in which an individual displays a special talent in one or more areas. The individual must exhibit exceptional abilities in one or more areas that are inconsistent with their overall level of functioning [1-3]. The most frequently used adjectives to define savant syndrome are “rare” and “remarkable,” indicating that this is a very rare condition characterized by remarkable talent.
There are two conditions that are essential for defining or diagnosing savant syndrome: “exceptional abilities” and “intellectual abilities.” “Exceptional ability” means that an individual shows extraordinary achievement in one or more areas. In other words, the individual demonstrates a special talent despite having a disability, even when compared to the general population. The second condition is intellectual ability. Previous studies have defined savant syndrome as a condition in which a special talent is demonstrated by a person with an intellectual disability. However, subsequent cases and studies have shown occurrences of savant syndrome even among individuals without any intellectual disability and other neurological disabilities as well as among the general population [2-5]. Howlin et al. [6] reported that the savant group displayed a higher level of overall cognitive function than the non-savant group and that their average non-verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) was approximately 80, with none of the individuals showing a non-verbal IQ below 50. These findings suggest that savant syndrome was observed even in groups without intellectual disabilities.
Savant syndromeshould be differentiated from the following terms that have similar meanings: exceptional ability, giftedness, and twice-exceptional [7]. Exceptional ability refers to skills that are exceptional compared to one’s overall cognitive functioning or even when compared to the general population, and this expression can have similar meanings to “exceptional skills” or “savant skills.” Giftedness refers to displaying a high level of performance compared to the same age group or a group with similar experiences. Twice-exceptional refers to an individual having two special traits: autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and enhanced perceptual functioning (EPF) in a particular area [7]. Displaying exceptional abilities in a particular area can also be explained by the concept of intraindividual variation, which has often been used as a diagnostic criterion for learning disabilities [2].
The current literature indicates that the characteristics of savant syndrome, ASD and savant syndrome, and cognitive features of people with ASD and savant syndrome have been studied extensively worldwide; however, there is a limited number of studies in Korea, with the only available studies being the clinical study by Kim et al. [8] and parent interviews conducted by Park [1]. Despite the scarcity of academic research on this topic in Korea, the illustration of savant syndrome by the mass media has raised awareness among the general population and sparked academic interest among relevant experts. The recently aired TV drama Extraordinary Attorney Woo [9] and film Nocturne [10] adequately directed the attention of the general population and relevant experts toward people with ASD and savant syndrome. Despite the growing popularity and interest in savant syndrome, there is a dearth of basic research on the specific characteristics of savant skills, the overall cognitive characteristics of individuals with ASD who possess these skills, and the understanding of having these skills among individuals themselves. Parents of children with ASD and savant syndrome have experienced various challenges and confusion in navigating the unfamiliar territory between ASD and savant syndrome, and have voiced the need for more systematic research on savant syndrome [1].
As documented in several studies, savant syndrome is more prevalent in individuals with ASD compared to other clinical populations, and for this reason, studies have continued to investigate which traits of ASD contribute to the onset of savant syndrome [6]. One hypothesis posits that savant skills may be related to the restricted repetitive behaviors (RRBs), special interests, and obsessive-compulsive traits, the second diagnostic criterion for ASD, while another theory proposes that the development of savant skills may stem from the difficulties in social cognitive skills, that is, difficulties in understanding others’ mind [11-13]. In addition, another hypothesis based on several study findings explains that the special cognitive features of individuals with ASD may contribute to the higher prevalence of savant skills [14-17]. Indeed, there is no single theory that explains the high prevalence of savant syndrome in individuals with ASD. Thus, it is important to conduct a systematic review of the existing literature to examine the research trends for people with ASD and savant syndrome and to explore the directions of subsequent research.
The present study was conducted to address the following objectives: 1) to analyze the research trends in individuals with ASD and savant syndrome over the past 15 years; 2) to examine the main characteristics of savant syndrome and the general characteristics of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome; and 3) to identify the cognitive characteristics of individuals with autism and savant skills.
This study analyzed studies published over the past 15 years to examine research trends in individuals with ASD and savant syndrome. Studies were included if they had been published over the past 15 years (2008–2022) in peer-reviewed journals, and were journal-published articles investigating the cognitive characteristics of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome.
Studies were excluded based on the following: studies that examined savant syndrome only in the general population; studies that examined other complex disabilities; studies examining biological factors, such as biochemistry, genetics, and molecular biology; studies dealing with other disabilities, such as epilepsy; and publications that were not peer reviewed, such as books, editorials, communications, conference presentations, and degree dissertations.
To ensure systematic literature analysis, the preferred reporting items for the systematic review method and procedure were followed [18]. The search strategy and process are described below.
An initial search was conducted by entering a combination of search words into the search database selected for the literature search. The keyword and a combination of keywords used for the literature search was “savant syndrome,” “ASD,” “savanna,” “autism,” and “cognition.” The following databases were searched: Scopus, Science Direct, PubMed, ISI Web, CINAHL Plus with Full Text (EBSCOhost), ERIC (ProQuest), Google Scholar, DBpia, and RISS.
Next, for a thorough data search, an initial search was performed for each database, and duplicates were removed. The titles and abstracts of the remaining articles (n=137) were then reviewed to exclude ineligible articles according to the purpose of the study and the inclusion and exclusion criteria.
Third, the remaining articles (n=86) were analyzed based on the variables selected for analysis, and articles that met the exclusion criteria were excluded. The basic exclusion criteria were studies conducted on individuals with neurological disabilities other than ASD, studies conducted only on the general population, and studies that mainly examined cranial and biological factors. Additionally, while reviewing the articles, studies without clearly defined methods or procedures or non-peer reviewed studies, such as editorials, were excluded.
Finally, 43studies were identified through this process. Subsequently, additional eligible studies were identified through the references, and studies that met the exclusion criteria, such as neuroimaging or cranial nerve-related studies, were excluded. Consequently, 40 studies were included in this review.
The aim of this study was to analyze the research trends regarding people with ASD and savant syndrome and their cognitive features. The studies were analyzed based on the variables selected from previous studies and for the purpose of the present study–publication year, study methodology, study objectives, study population, measurements, and study outcomes [19-22].
The included studies were analyzed according to the study methodology, and divided into literature reviews and surveys and different variables were used for the two types of study methodologies. In the literature review, the study objectives, topics, and results were analyzed. Survey studies were analyzed according to their purpose, participants, measurement and investigation methods, and results. Survey research was categorized into case studies, experimental studies, and questionnaire studies according to the research execution methods; one study that performed a qualitative interview was also included.
Research trends by publication year and methodology
In this study,a total of 40 articles were analyzed by research methods and publication year. A review according to the research method showed 20 literature reviews and 20 survey studies. Two studies were published in Korea and 38 were published in an English-speaking country. Table 1 lists the studies according to the publication year and methods. In terms of quantitative changes in studies on the cognitive characteristics of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome by year, the highest number of studies was published in 2009, with eight literature reviews and three survey studies, while related studies are being conducted to date. The reason for the highest volume of research in 2009 appears to be because of the coverage of ASD and savant syndrome in Volume 364, Issue 1522 of Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, which inspired academic research on this topic.
Themes and subthemes of literature reviews
Over the past15 years, 20 studies have explored theories pertinent to the overall and cognitive characteristics of individuals with ASD exhibiting savant syndrome. These 20 exploratory studies were analyzed for overall trends based on the study themes. As shown in Table 2, five themes and seven subthemes were derived.
Table 3 presents the overall outline of the literature reviews. The main themes of this study are as follows: 1) basic theories about savant syndrome and its association with ASD [3,5,23-25]; 2) incidence of savant syndrome, sex differences, and their relationship with overall cognitive functioning [5,26]; 3) type and extent of savant skills [5,26]; 4) the relationship between ASD and savant syndrome [23,25-30]; and 5) the cognitive characteristics of individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome. The studies conducted on the cognitive characteristics primarily examined weak central coherence (WCC) and detail-focused cognitive processing [13,25-28,31], rule-based pattern recognition, hyper-systemizing, enhanced perceptual abilities and synesthesia, deficits in theory of mind, RRBs, and attentional issues [25-27,32-39] (Table 3).
Overview of survey research
Table 4 presents the overall survey research trends. Twenty survey research studies were categorized into six case studies (including a questionnaire survey of 125 parents) (23.8%), nine experimental studies (47.6%), five questionnaire studies (one study included three cases and was considered a duplicate case) (23.8%), and one qualitative interview study (4.8%).
Study participants
Regarding numberof study participants, there were six case studies (28.6%) with a small number of participants of ≤10 (one of these studies conducted in-depth interviews with parents), five questionnaire studies with a relatively small sample of ≤50 (23.8%) (one study included three cases and was considered duplicate cases), six studies conducted on 51–200 participants (28.6%), and four larger studies conducted on a sample of ≥200 (19.0%).
In one questionnaire survey conducted with more than 100 parents, data were collected using existing data in the database.
The ages of the participants varied widely from children to adults. The youngest patient was 7 years old, and the oldest was 90 years old. However, most studies examined participants in their late teens up to approximately 40 years of age.
Objective
The objectives of the included studies were primarily to compare individuals with ASD and savant syndrome and those with ASD without savant syndrome or to compare individuals with ASD and savant syndrome with the general population to examine the differences in cognitive processing, cognitive traits, and clinical features in individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome. More specifically, the present study aimed to examine the relationship between enhanced perception, execution functions, exceptional talent, and autistic tendencies, and to compare individuals with ASD who possess savant skills and those who do not possess savant skills to identify the cognitive factors that contribute to savant skills.
Themes of survey research
Five themes and eight subthemes were identified in the survey studies (Tables 2 and 4). The main results of the analysis are as follows. First, there were studies that analyzed the demographic characteristics of individuals with ASD who displayed savant syndrome. These studies showed that the incidence of savant syndrome in the ASD population was higher than that in the general population or in other patient populations with neurodevelopmental disabilities. However, the incidence of savant syndrome was inconsistent, ranging from 10% to 50%. The prevalence of savant syndrome was higher in men than in women [6,8]. Second, intellectual ability was significantly higher in the group with savant syndrome than in the group without savant syndrome [6,8,13,17,40]. Third, the most common types of savant skills were memorization, music, art, reading, spatiotemporal, mathematics, and calendar calculations, and many individuals showed two or more savant skills [8,12,41]. Fourth, studies have investigated the differences in social communication and RRBs, the core diagnostic criteria for ASD, to analyze the relationship between savant syndrome and ASD. The results showed that the two groups did not clearly show variances in the social communication area but did show differences in RRBs. Thus, the savant syndrome group tends to exhibit more RRBs than the non-savant syndrome group [6,8,11,12]. Daniel and Menashe [11] suggested that a group with savant syndrome exhibited fewer deficits in social responsiveness than a group without savant syndrome. These results are in line with findings suggesting that those with savant syndrome have a higher over-all intellectual ability. However, Bennett and Rebedew [42] stated that exceptional talent may be linked to exceptional attention to detail rather than RRBs, including obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Fifth, cognitive traits that contribute to savant syndrome among individuals with ASD have been proposed to employ cognitive processing modalities, such as excellent working memory [42-44], enhanced executive functioning [15], EPF, synesthesia [45-47], mechanical memorization, and systemizing [15,17,48]. Lastly, their unique cognitive approaches have been observed to result in swifter and more sophisticated task performance [12,17,48], more rule-based task performance [16,27,31], and consequently higher response accuracy [16,49].
What is savant syndrome?
Providingan accurate depiction of the characteristics of savant syndrome can be challenging, given the vast array of areas and extent of savant skills. Nevertheless, various studies have elucidated the common characteristics of this condition.
First, savant syndrome is a rare condition; however, the exceptional capabilities displayed by individuals with savant syndrome are remarkable. Savant syndrome can be found in all populations with neurological deficits, including ASD and intellectual disabilities, as well as in the general population. The prevalence of savant syndrome is the highest in the ASD population, with approximately 50% of all individuals with savant syndrome having ASD [50]. Additionally, there is a higher incidence of savant syndrome among males, with a reported male to female ratio of 6:1 [5] or 4:1 [32,41]. It has been suggested that this male predominance may be attributed to left-hemisphere sensitivity and the influence of the male sex hormone testosterone [14,51].
According to the results of our literature analysis, few studies have reported the incidence of savant syndrome and sex ratios for the incidence rate; however, these data remain unclear. This may be attributable to the lack of diagnostic criteria or systems for savant syndrome and the difficulty in developing screening or diagnostic tools focused on a particular area of talent, as the areas and degrees of skill in savant syndrome vary widely [24].
Second, individuals with savant syndrome often display exceptional talent in two or more areas. They tend to show extraordinary memory in addition to the exceptional talents in other major areas [5,8]. Although memorization is extraordinary, it may seem automatic and unconscious. Often, expressions such as “verbal adhesion,” “automatic,” and “exaggerated form of memory” are used to describe this unique memory skill [8]. In other words, the memory skill of individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome may pertain more to automatic memory, that is, implicit memory than “meaning” [27].
Third, although savant syndrome is mostly congenital, cases of acquired savant syndrome as a result of brain injury have also been reported [24,26]. The notion that savant syndrome is congenital is based on cases in which individuals exhibit exceptional talent despite a lack of special education or experience. Even this congenital condition may advance further through practice and training beyond a certain age. According to a Korean study, nine mothers of children with savant syndrome recalled that their children were born with savant skills but that the skills became more advanced through education [1]. In contrast, there are also rare cases of acquired savant skills after an accident or disease that injures a specific part of the brain, such as the left parietal lobe [24]. These individuals developed savant skills that they did not possess prior to the brain injury; hence, the term acquired savant skills.
Fourth, savant skills tend to be advanced and not diminished or lost [5]. However, similar to the well-known case of Nadia, there are cases in which savant skills are suddenly lost. Nadia exhibited a prodigious talent for drawing in early childhood, which vanished after intensive learning of the language. This account has prompted discussions on the phenomenon of special talent diminishing or disappearing following the acquisition of a language or education. Nonetheless, such instances are exceedingly rare, and numerous studies have reported that savant skills are more constructively advanced as children grow [5,26]. A study conducted in Korea reported that parents observed that their children’s exceptional abilities became more pronounced as they grew older. However, there were also parents who said that their children with ASD who showed savant syndrome experienced a temporary decline in their artistic talent as their social skills improved, while their social interaction deteriorated once they immersed themselves in drawing again [1]. In summary, while savant skills may not diminish or disappear, increased opportunities for social interaction with others could reduce the time spent practicing these skills, which can be considered as a temporary regression of talent.
Savant syndrome should be recognized as a spectrum
Savant syndrome is not a uniform phenomenon. It is a multifaceted condition with varying areas and extent of skills involved; thus, it should be viewed as a spectrum. These skills are most commonly observed in areas such as music, art, calendar calculations, mathematics, visuospatial, memory, linguistic abilities, and sensory sensitivity but may also be observed in other areas [3,6,12]. While savant skills can manifest in a single domain, they can also be observed across multiple domains, and those with two or more savant skills often show extraordinary memory [41]. Bal et al. [52] indicated that 46% of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome possess two or more savant skills. Similarly, Korean studies have reported that individuals with ASD who demonstrate savant skills often exhibit these skills across multiple domains [1,8].
Further, savant skills should be understood as a spectrum because the extent of savant skills varies significantly. While many people perceive savant skills as exceptional and remarkable, limited splinter skills fall under the category of savant skills among individuals with ASD. For example, a splinter skill such as the ability to memorize all subway stations may also be considered savant. Individuals with savant skills can be classified into three types based on their degree of exceptionality [3,5]: 1) prodigious savants, a rare group of savants who demonstrate phenomenal talent, even among the general population; 2) talented savants who demonstrate marked talent in a particular area, such as music and art, compared to their overall abilities; and 3) some individuals demonstrate splinter skills such as remembering license plates, maps, historical events, or particular songs as standalone memories.
Diagnosis of savant syndrome
As discussed thus far, despite the fact that savant syndrome has been defined and evidence has been accumulated in the literature, specific and systematic definitions and diagnostic criteria for savant syndrome are still lacking. In particular, the lack of official diagnostic criteria or any relevant information on savant syndrome in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM) or International Classification of Diseases (ICD) shows that savant syndrome is not recognized as a specific clinical domain [24].
Several screening tools have been developed to treat savant syndrome. For example, Hughes et al. [12] developed the Sussex Cognitive Style Questionnaire to assess areas of savant skills, and Bennett and Heaton [42] developed a questionnaire to screen special skills among individuals with ASD. Although the use of such screening tools is relatively promising, there are still limitations, in that there are no validated and standardized instruments. Amid such challenges in the diagnosis of savant syndrome, items 88–93 of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), the gold standard for ASD diagnosis, are commonly used to understand savant skills [53].
WCC and detail-focused cognitive processors
Central coherence refers to the capacityto establish meaningful connections holistically and assimilate information from the external environment. Research has shown that individuals with ASD exhibit weaker central coherence than those without ASD. In other words, while people with ASD demonstrate an exceptional capacity for processing localized and detailed information, they frequently encounter challenges in integrating individual details or establishing meaningful connections between information pertaining to a broader context, owing to their reduced central coherence [54]. The concept of central coherence explains how individuals process cognition and is based on field theory, which was previously employed in the context of learning cognition. Field theory categorizes learners as field-dependent and field-independent based on their cognitive characteristics. Field-dependent learners tend to perceive external input as a whole and excel in identifying and synthesizing the flow and gist of the given information. In contrast, field-independent learners tend to have difficulty actively utilizing social context or surrounding factors when processing information. As a result, they tend to focus on the individual parts and details of the given information [54].
There is extensive evidence to indicate that individuals with ASD have WCC [23,54]. Such individuals find it challenging to select the necessary information from complex informational sources, meaningfully integrate and utilize the information, and process intricate information. In addition, people with ASD often have difficulty summarizing, selecting, and remembering key points from various messages and pieces of information that they need to learn. As a result, while they have no issues remembering particular aspects of the given information, they have difficulty comprehending the main themes or the overarching narrative of the story. Moreover, individuals with ASD may struggle to comprehend and systematically analyze various types of information.
The WCC theory, which has been understood as a cognitive deficiency, has been utilized to explain the occurrence of savant syndrome in individuals with ASD. According to this theory, individuals with ASD tend to engage in analytical thinking when processing external information, resulting in a focus on specific details, and enabling them to develop cognitive styles that allow them to excel in more detailed areas [23]. In other words, individuals with ASD who have savant syndrome exhibit remarkable abilities in particular areas, and this theory attributes this to WCC or a detail-focused cognitive processing style [30]. The primary areas of exceptional talent associated with detail-focused cognitive processing are mathematics, music, and the art [55,56]. Because of this detail- focused cognitive processing, individuals with ASD tend to concentrate on specific details in areas such as music, technology, and mathematics and, as a result, can acquire special abilities that exceed those of the general population [23,57].
Bennett and Heaton [43] examined 17 patients with ASD, 19 non-savants with ASD, and 17 individuals without ASD or savant syndrome, to determine the cognitive phenotypes of savants with ASD. Specifically, they investigated whether savant syndrome is associated with autistic tendencies, namely, special cognitive styles such as detail-focused cognitive processing and systemizing. Their results indicated that savants scored higher on working memory and analytic skills than non-savants [25,43]. In fact, detail-focused cognitive processors exhibit two-fold more exceptional talent than their counterparts [13]. In essence, the cognitive phenotypes observed among individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome, namely obsessive tendencies, excellent working memory, analytic skills, and detail-focused cognitive processing styles, may be involved in the development of savant skills.
Therefore, the special abilities of individuals with ASD are believed to be linked to detail-focused cognitive processing. Individuals with ASD can focus more on details or special areas or domains than the general population, which drives them to demonstrate extraordinary talent in relevant areas [54,56]. Thus, cognitive processing can be understood in terms of detail-focused processing, which pertains to strength, as opposed to WCC, which pertains to weakness.
EPF and hyper-systemizing
EPF is a useful theory for explaining the exceptional capabilities of students with ASD, beyond other theories that explained the cognitive phenotypes of students with ASD based on WCC [31,54,55]. The EPF model stresses that automatic perception and enhanced roles as the major cognitive features of individuals with ASD [26,36,56]. Individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome automatic and enhanced cognitive processes during perceptual information processing, which contribute to the development of exceptional abilities. For example, savant skills, such as synesthesia, perfect pitch, and hyperlexia, share similar neural structures that can be understood as EPF.
As shown in the literature, EPF is one of the most frequently discussed theories in explaining the cognitive traits of individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome [15,17,34,35]. Although EPF may be in line with WCC theory in that it also features detail-focused perceptual traits, WCC theory posits the inability to view a concept as a whole as a deficit, whereas the EPF model understands the presence of low-level perceptual operations as a factor of exceptionality [54].
According to previous studies on the visual and auditory perceptions of individuals with ASD, these individuals demonstrate better performance in low-level cognitive operations such as discrimination. In other words, individuals with ASD excel in visual and auditory perceptual processes compared to sociality and higher-level cognitive processing, and such cognitive pattern is believed to be attributed to the hyper-activation of the parts of the brain related to primary perceptual functions [34,35,57]. The co-occurrence of target detection tasks, such as mnemonic, attentional, or visuospatial operations, and remarkable discrimination and co-occurrence of perfect pitch and excellent auditory memory among individuals with ASD may constitute examples of EPF [58].
Pring et al. [17] administered a block design test and an embedded figure test to test the hypothesis that EPF is a common characteristic in individuals with ASD and savant syndrome. Their results indicated that the savant and control groups did not differ in the embedded figure test, but the savant group performed better in the block design test than the other groups. The block design test involves constructional tasks, and these results support the notion that EPF is involved. Although several studies have presented evidence supporting the theory that EPF is a common feature of individuals with ASD and savant skills, the extent to which EPF is involved in the development of savant skills remains unknown.
Mottron et al. [34] stated that various mediating factors such as age and level of experience in the area could affect exceptional visual and auditory perception among individuals with ASD. In other words, enhanced visual discrimination advances spatiotemporal abilities, whereas enhanced auditory discrimination contributes to remarkable musical talent. Although the EPF model explains the exceptional visual and auditory processing styles among individuals with ASD to some degree, “enhanced pattern perception” better explains savant skills [34].
A comparison of savants with ASD, non-savants with ASD, and the normal control group revealed that the group with ASD and savant syndrome had higher sensory sensitivity, tended to be more obsessive, engaged in hypersystemizing, and employed a locally oriented cognitive style [43]. These characteristics may contribute to the development of savant skills. Individuals with ASD who exhibit savant syndrome pay attention to details and tend to hypersystematize. For instance, their ability to recognize rules such as “q” follows “p” is augmented, and such ability contributes to the development of exceptional talents [12,14]. As previously mentioned, high sensory sensitivity, obsessive-compulsive behaviors, a locally oriented cognitive style, exceptional working memory, and analytic skills stimulate vertical mapping and pattern recognition, ultimately contributing to the development of savant skills [43].
This study examined the research trends regarding individuals with ASD and savant syndrome over the past 15 years and explored the main characteristics of savant syndrome as well as the major phenomena and cognitive phenotypes of individuals with ASD who possess savant skills. Our findings indicate that there has been continued research on individuals with ASD and savant syndrome over the past 15 years, and the study methodology can be broadly divided into literature reviews and survey research studies. Five main themes and nine subthemes were identified. The five main themes were: 1) definition and characteristics of savant syndrome, 2) demographic characteristics of savants, 3) types and extent of savant skills, 4) savant syndrome and ASD, and 5) cognitive profiles of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome.
Based on these findings, we present the following points for further discussion. First, most theoretical exploratory studies have focused on establishing fundamental theoretical grounds such as the concept of savant syndrome and the characteristics of individuals with savant syndrome. These results reflect the lack of theoretical consensus on ASD and savant syndrome. Furthermore, the fact that all relevant studies were either theoretical explorations or surveys calls for further studies with diverse methodologies. In other words, studies should investigate whether savant skills can be advanced through education, or whether the cognitive functions of individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome can be improved through education or external support. Although this study limited the subject of the literature for research purposes, an initial overview of the basic data prior to the literature analysis showed that there were insufficient studies that conducted interventions or treatments on savant skills and examined their effectiveness. These results suggest that comprehensive research on ASD and savant syndrome has not yet been conducted.
Second, as suggested by our results, the types and extent of savant syndrome are diverse and multifaceted to the extent that it may be construed as a spectrum. Thus, there is no single criterion for diagnosing savant syndrome nor has it been included in a reliable diagnostic framework [59,60]. These findings underscore the need to establish a diagnostic system that accounts for the definition of savant syndrome and the type and severity of savant skills [61].
Third, survey research studies either had a small sample size, including case series, or had a large sample size, which analyzed data from a database. In terms of age, most studies included all age groups, from children to adults, as opposed to specifying a particular age group. This means that the participant characteristics were not controlled for, which may suggest that savant syndrome is a relatively rare condition. Although savant syndrome is more prevalent in the ASD population than in other populations, there is still difficulty in sampling enough participants to conduct a well-controlled study.
Fourth, studies have explained the cognitive phenotypes of individuals with ASD who display savant syndrome in terms of EPF, detail-focused cognitive processing style, hypersystemizing, or pattern recognition [27]. Several different theories have been used to explain the cognitive features of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome; however, these theories influence one another or share common characteristics in significant ways, as opposed to being mutually exclusive.
In conclusion, the characteristics of individuals with ASD who exhibit savant syndrome can be explained by various factors. However, as noted by Treffert and Rebedew [41], there is no single theory that can account for savant syndrome, nor is there a single theory that can specify the cognitive phenotypes of individuals with ASD and savant syndrome. Therefore, further systematic and multidimensional research is needed on individuals with ASD and savant syndrome [62].
Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the study.
The author has no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
This study was supported by the Ministry of Education of the Republic of Korea and National Research Foundation of Korea (grant number NRF-2021S1A5A2A01069519).